References

Adams, F. & Aizawa, K. (2008). The bounds of cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Arel, I., Rose, D. & Karnowski, T. (2010). Deep machine learning - A new frontier in artificial intelligence research. IEEE Computational Intelligence Magazine, 5 (4), 13-18. 10.1109/MCI.2010.938364

Bateson, G. (1973). Steps to an ecology of mind. London, UK: Paladin.

Beauchamp, M. S. & Martin, A. (2007). Grounding object concepts in perception and action: Evidence from fMRI studies of tools. Cortex, 43 (3), 461-468. 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70470-2

Bengio, Y. (2009). Learning deep architectures for AI. Foundations & Trends in Machine Learning, 2 (1), 1-127. 10.1561/2200000006

Brandom, R. B. (2008). Between saying and doing: Towards an analytic pragmatism. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Brooks, R. A. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47 (1-3), 139-159. 10.1016/0004-3702(91)90053-M

Buonomano, D. V. & Merzenich, M. M. (1998). Cortical plasticity: From synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 21, 149-186. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.21.1.149

Chiel, H. J. & Beer, R. D. (1997). The brain has a body: Adaptive behavior emerges from interactions of nervous system, body, and environment. Trends in Neuroscience, 20 (12), 553-557. 10.1016/S0166-2236(97)01149-1

Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58 (1), 7-19. 10.1093/analys/58.1.7

Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. London, UK: MIT Press.

(2001). Mindware: An introduction to the philosophy of cognitive science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Crafa, D. & Nagel, S. K. (forthcoming). Traces of culture: The feedback loop between brain, behavior, and disorder. Transcultural Psychiatry

De Jaegher, H. & Di Paolo, E. A. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6 (4), 485-507.

Dennett, D. C. (2000). Making tools for thinking. In D. Sperber (Ed.) Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 17-29). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York, NY: Penguin-Putnam Inc..

(1991). Experience and nature. In J. A. Boydston (Ed.) The later works of John Dewey (Vol. 1). Edwardsville, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

Dreyfus, H. (1992). What computers still can’t do: A critique of artificial reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Engel, A. K., Fries, P. & Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 (10), 704-716. 10.1038/35094565

Engel, A. K., Maye, A., Kurthen, M. & König, P. (2013). Where's the action? The pragmatic turn in cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17 (5), 202-209.

Freeman, W. J. (1995). Societies of brains: A study in the neuroscience of love and hate. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Friston, K. J. & Stephan, K. E. (2007). Free energy and the brain. Synthese, 159, 417-458. 10.1007/s11229-007-9237-y

Friston, K. J., Daunizeau, J., Kilner, J. & Kiebel, S. J. (2010). Action and behavior: A free-energy formulation. Biological Cybernetics, 102 (3), 227-260. 10.1007/s00422-010-0364-z

Fuchs, T. (2009). Embodied cognitive neuroscience and its consequences for psychiatry. Poiesis and Praxis, 6 (3-4), 219-233. 10.1007/s10202-008-0068-9

(2011). The brain – a mediating organ. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 18 (7-8), 196-221.

(2012). Are mental illnesses diseases of the brain? In S. Choudhury & J. Slaby (Eds.) Critical neuroscience: A handbook of the social and cultural contexts of neuroscience (pp. 331-344). Malden, MA: Wiley: Blackwell.

Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Graziano, M. S. A., Yap, G. S. & Charles, G. G. (1994). Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science, 266, 1054-1057. 10.1126/science.7973661

Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27 (3), 377-442. 10.1017/S0140525X04000093

Haken, H. (2004). Synergetics, introduction and advanced topics. Berlin, GER: Springer.

Hinton, G. E., Osindero, S. & Teh, Y. (2006). A fast learning algorithm for deep belief nets. Neural Computation, 18 (7), 1527-1554. 10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1527

Hohwy, J., Roepstorff, A. & Friston, K. J. (2008). Predictive coding explains binocular rivalry: An epistemological review. Cognition, 108 (3), 687-701. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.010

Ingold, T. (1990). An anthropologist looks at biology. Man, New Series, 25 (2), 208-229.

(1993). Tool-use, sociality and intelligence. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.) Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 429-445). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

(2001). From the transmission of representations to the education of attention. In H. Whitehouse (Ed.) The debated mind: Evolutionary psychology versus ethnography (pp. 113-153). Berg: Oxford, UK.

(2004). Beyond biology and culture: The meaning of evolution in a relational world. Social Anthropology, 12 (2), 209-221. 10.1111/j.1469-8676.2004.tb00102.x

Johnson, M. (2007). The meaning of the body: Aesthetics of human understanding. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Jordan, J. S. & Day, B. (2015). Wild systems theory as a 21st century coherence framework for cognitive science. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.) Open MIND (pp. 1-21). Frankfurt a. M., GER: MIND Group.

Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Keyser, J. & Nagel, S. K. (2014). Stimulating more than the patient's brain: Deep brain stimulation from a systems perspective. American Journal of Bioethics, 5 (4), 60-62. 10.1080/21507740.2014.953268

Kitcher, P. S. (2012). Preludes to pragmatism: Toward a reconstruction of philosophy. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and Its challenge to western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Lewis, M. D. (2005). Bridging emotion theory and neurobiology through dynamic systems modeling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28 (2), 169-245. 10.1017/S0140525X0500004X

Lutz, A. & Thompson, E. (2003). Neurophenomenology: Integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10 (9-10), 31-52.

Maturana, H. R. & Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. Dordrecht, NL: Reidel.

Mecacci, G. & Haselager, W. F. G. (2014). Stimulating the self: The influence of conceptual frameworks on reactions to deep brain stimulation. AJOB Neuroscience, 5 (4), 30-39. 10.1080/21507740.2014.951776

Menary, R. (2007). Cognitive integration: Mind and cognition unbounded. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan..

(2010). Dimensions of mind. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9, 561-578.

Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press.

Oyama, S. (2000). Evolution’s eye: A systems view of the biology-culture divide. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

O’Regan, J. K. & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24 (5), 939-1031. 10.1017/S0140525X01000115

Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F. & Merabet, L. B. (2005). The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 377-401. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144216

Rao, R. P. N & Ballard, D. H (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 79-87. 10.1038/4580

Rupert, R. (2009). Cognitive systems and the extended mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Schulkin, J. (2009). Cognitive adaptation: A pragmatist perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Shapiro, L. (2011). Embodied Cognition. New York, NY: Routledge.

Sporns, O. (2011). Networks of the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Sprevak, M. (2009). Extended cognition and functionalism. The Journal of Philosophy, 106 (9), 503-527.

Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Thompson, E. (2010). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Torrance, S. (2009). Contesting the concept of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16 (5), 111-126.

Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3 (4), 330-350.

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E. & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Vohs, K. D. & Schooler, J. W. (2008). The value of believing in free will: Encouraging a belief in determinism increases cheating. Psychological Science, 19 (1), 49-54. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02045.x

von Uexküll, J. (1940). Bedeutungslehre. Semiotica, 42 (1), 25-82.

Ward, D. & Stapleton, M. (2012). Es are good: Cognition as enacted, embodied, embedded, affective and extended. In F. Paglieri (Ed.) Consciousness in interaction: The role of the natural and social context in shaping consciousness (pp. 89-104). Amsterdam, NL: John Benjamins.

Wheeler, M. (2011). Embodied cognition and the extended mind. In J. Garvey (Ed.) The continuum companion to philosophy of mind (pp. 220-238). London, UK: Continuum.

Wilson, M. & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131 (3), 460-473. 10.1037/0033-2909.131.3.460