References

Arango-Muñoz, S. (2012). The nature of epistemic feelings. Philosophical Psychology, 27 (2), 193-211. 10.1080/09515089.2012.732002

Barrett, L. F. & Bar, M. (2009). See it with feeling: Affective predictions during object perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364 (1521), 1325-1334. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0312.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10 (3), 295-307. 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295.

Beran, M. J., Brandl, J., Perner, J. & Proust, J. (Eds.) (2012). The foundations of metacognition. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Botvinick, M. & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands 'feel' touch that eyes see. Nature, 391 (6669), 756-756. 10.1038/35784.

Brown, A. S. (1991). A review of the tip-of-the-tongue experience. Psychological Bulletin, 109 (2), 204-223. 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.204

Carruthers, P. (2011). The opacity of mind. An integrative theory of self-knowledge. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97 (1), 19-35. 10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19

(2001). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York, NY: Putnam.

(2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow and the feeling brain. Orlando, FL: Harvest Book Harcourt, Inc.

Decety, J. & Meyer, M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account. Development and Psychopathology, 20 (04), 1053-1080. 10.1017/S0954579408000503

Dezecache, G., Conty, L., Chadwick, M., Philip, L., Soussignan, R., Sperber, D. & Grèzes, J. (2013). Evidence for unintentional emotional contagion beyond dyads. PLoS One, 8 (6), e67371-e67371. 10.1371/journal.pone.0067371

Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298 (5596), 1191-1194. 10.1126/science.1076358

Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6 (3-4), 45-60. 10.1080/02699939208411068

Fleming, S. M. & Dolan, R. J. (2012). The neural basis of metacognitive ability. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367 (1594), 1338-1349. 10.1098/rstb.2011.0417

Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reasons: The strategic role of the emotions. New York, NY: Norton.

Goldie, P. (2002). The emotions: A philosophical exploration. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

(2009). Getting feelings into emotional experience in the right way. Emotion Review, 1 (3), 232-239. 10.1177/1754073909103591

Gordon, R. (1987). The structure of emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Griffiths, P. E. & Scarantino, A. (2009). Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion. In M. Aydede & P. Robbins (Eds.) The Cambridge handbook of situated cognition (pp. 437-453). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Griffiths, P. E. (1997). What emotions really are. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.

(2004). Emotions as natural and normative kinds. Philosophy of Science, 71 (5), 901-911. 10.1086/425944

Haggard, P., Clark, S. & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 282-285. 10.1038/nn827

Hume, D. (2007). Treatise of human nature. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.

Jackson, P. L., Meltzoff, A. N. & Decety, J. (2005). How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy. NeuroImage, 24 (3), 771-779. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006

Jacoby, L. L. & Brooks, L. R. (1984). Nonanalytic cognition: Memory, perception, and concept learning. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 18, 1-47. 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60358-8

Jacoby, L. L. & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition infuenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118 (2), 126-135. 10.1037/0096-3445.118.2.126

James, W. (1884/1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Dover.

Kelley, C. M. & Jacoby, L. L. (1998). Subjective reports and process dissociation: Fluency, knowing, and feeling. Acta Psychologica, 98 (2), 127-140. 10.1016/S0001-6918(97)00039-5

Kenny, A. (1963). Action, emotion and will. London, UK: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Kepecs, A., Naoshige, U., Zariwala, H. & Mainen, Z. F. (2008). Neural correlates, computation and behavioural impact of decision confidence. Nature, 455, 227-231. 10.1038/nature07200

Koriat, A. & Ackerman, R. (2010). Metacognition and mindreading: Judgments of learning for self and other during self-paced study. Consciousness and Cognition, 19 (1), 251-264. 10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.010

Koriat, A. & Levy-Sadot, R. (1999). Processes underlying metacognitive judgments: Information-based and experience-based monitoring of one's own knowledge. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.) Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 483-502). London, UK: The Guilford Press.

(2001). The combined contributions of the cue-familiarity and accessibility heuristics to feelings of knowing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27 (1), 34-34. 10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.34.

Koriat, A. (2000). The feeling of knowing: Some metatheoretical implications for consciousness and control. Consciousness and Cognition, 9 (2), 149-171. 10.1006/ccog.2000.0433

Lamme, V. A. & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23 (11), 571-579. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-X

LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.

Logan, G. D. & Crump, M. J. (2010). Cognitive illusions of authorship reveal hierarchical error detection in skilled typists. Science, 330 (6004), 683-686. 10.1126/science.1190483

Mangan, B. (1993). Taking phenomenology seriously: The "fringe" and its implications for cognitive research. Consciousness and Cognition, 2 (2), 89-108. 10.1006/ccog.1993.1008

(2000). What feeling is the "feeling of knowing"? Consciousness and Cognition, 9 (4), 516-537. 10.1006/ccog.2000.0488

Murata, A., Moser, J. S. & Kitayama, S. (2013). Culture shapes electrocortical responses during emotion suppression. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8 (5), 595-601. 10.1093/scan/nss036

Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38 (5), 752-760. 10.1111/1469-8986.3850752

Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological review, 84 (3), 231-259.

Nussinson, R. & Koriat, A. (2008). Correcting experience-based judgments: The perseverance of subjective experience in the face of the correction of judgment. Metacognition and Learning, 3 (2), 159-174. 10.1007/s11409-008-90

Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 (6), 237-241. 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.014

Ortony, A., Clore, G. L. & Foss, M. A. (1987). The referential structure of the affective lexicon. Cognitive Science, 11 (3), 341-364. 10.1207/s15516709cog1103_4

Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual framework. Cognition, 107 (1), 179-217. 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.09.003

Plutchik, R. & Ax, A. F. (1967). A critique of determinants of emotional state by Schachter and Singer (1962). Psychophysiology, 4 (1), 79-82. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1967.tb02740.x

Prinz, J. (2004). Gut reactions. A perceptual theory of emotions. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Proust, J. (2000). Awareness of agency: Three levels of analysis. In T. Metzinger (Ed.) The neural correlates of consciousness (pp. 307-324). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(2009). The representational basis of brute metacognition: A proposal. In R. Lurz (Ed.) Philosophy of animal minds: New essays on animal thought and consciousness (pp. 165-183). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

(2013). The philosophy of metacognition. Mental agency and self-awareness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Reber, R., Fazendeiro, T. A. & Winkielman, P. (2002). Processing fluency as the source of experiences at the fringe of consciousness. Psyche, 8 (10), 1-21. 10.1155/6152

Reber, R., Schwarz, N. & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver's processing experience? Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8 (4), 364-382. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0804_3

Recanati, F. (2002). Does linguistic communication rest on inference? Mind & Language, 17 (1-2), 105-126. 10.1111/1468-0017.00191

Schwartz, B. L., Travis, D. M., Castro, A. M. & Smith, S. M. (2000). The phenomenology of real and illusory tip-of-the-tongue states. Memory & Cognition, 28 (1), 18-27. 10.3758/BF03211571

Schwarz, N. & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.) Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 385-407). New York, NY: Guilford.

Schwarz, N. (2004). Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14 (4), 332-348. 10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_2

Smith, E. R. & De Coster, J. (1999). Associative and rule based processing. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.) Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 323-336). New York, NY: Guilford.

Solomon, R. C. (2007). True to our feelings: What our emotions are really telling us. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Stepper, S. & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64 (2), 211-220. 10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.211

Strawson, P. F. (1959). Individuals. London, UK: Methuen.

Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J. A. & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63 (3), 107-140. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001

Tye, M. (2008). The experience of emotion: An intentionalist theory. Revue Internationale de Philosophie, 243 (1), 25-50.

Unkelbach, C. & Greifeneder, R. (2013). A general model of fluency effects in judgment and decision making. In C. Unkelbach & R. Greifeneder (Eds.) The experience of thinking: How the fluency of mental processes influences cognition and behavior (pp. 11-32). London, UK: Psychology Press.

Unkelbach, C. (2007). Reversing the truth effect: Learning the interpretation of processing fluency in judgments of truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33 (1), 219-230.

Whittlesea, B. W. & Williams, L. D. (2000). The source of feelings of familiarity: The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26 (3), 547-547. 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.547

(2001). The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: I. The heuristic basis of feelings and familiarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27 (1), 3-13. 10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.3

Winkielman, P. & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: Psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81 (6), 989-1000. 10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.989

Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Flanagan, J. R. (2001). Perspectives and problems in motor learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5 (11), 487-494. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01773-3

Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9 (2 pt 2), 1-27. 10.1037/h0025848

(1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35 (2), 151. 10.1037/0003-066X.35.2.151