References

Adams, W. J., Graf, E. W. & Ernst, M. O. (2004). Experience can change the 'light-from-above' prior. Nature Neuroscience, 7 (10), 1057-1058. 10.1038/nn1312

Block, N. & Siegel, S. (2013). Attention and perceptual adaptation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36 (3), 205-206. 10.1017/S0140525X12002245

Block, N. (1976). Troubles with functionalism. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 9, 261-325.

Braddon-Mitchell, D. & Jackson, F. (2006). The philosophy of mind and cognition: An introduction. London, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

Brown, H., Adams, R. A., Parees, I., Edwards, M. & Friston, K. (2013). Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions. Cognitive Processing, 14 (4), 411-427. 10.1007/s10339-013-0571-3

Chirimuuta, M. (2014). Minimal models and canonical neural computations: The distinctness of computational explanation in neuroscience. Synthese, 191 (2), 127-153. 10.1007/s11229-013-0369-y

Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 36 (3), 181-204. 10.1017/S0140525X12000477

Dretske, F. (1983). Knowledge and the flow of information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Feldman, H. & Friston, K. (2010). Attention, uncertainty and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4 (215), 1-23. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215

Friston, K. & Stephan, K. (2007). Free energy and the brain. Synthese, 159 (3), 417-458. 10.1007/s11229-007-9237-y

Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11 (2), 127-138. 10.1038/nrn2787

(2013). Life as we know it. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 10 (86). 10.1098/rsif.2013.0475

Friston, K., Thornton, C. & Clark, A. (2012). Free-energy minimization and the dark room problem. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (130), 1-7. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00130

Gregory, R. L. (1980). Perceptions as hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Biological Sciences, 290 (1038), 181-197. 10.1098/rstb.1980.0090

Harrison, L. M., Stephan, K. E., Rees, G. & Friston, K. J. (2007). Extra-classical receptive field effects measured in striate cortex with fMRI. NeuroImage, 34 (3), 1199-1208. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.017

Hinton, G. E. (2007). Learning multiple layers of representation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11 (10), 428-434. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.004

Hobson, J. A. & Friston, K. J. (2012). Waking and dreaming consciousness: Neurobiological and functional considerations. Progress in Neurobiology, 98 (1), 82-98. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.05.003

Hohwy, J. (2012). Attention and conscious perception in the hypothesis testing brain. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (96), 1-14. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00096

(2013). The predictive mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

(2014). The self-evidencing brain. Noûs, Early View. 10.1111/nous.12062

Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Kaplan, D. M. (2011). Explanation and description in computational neuroscience. Synthese, 183 (3), 339-373. 10.1007/s11229-011-9970-0

Kitcher, P. (1989). Explanatory unification and the causal structure of the world. In P. Kitcher & W. Salmon (Eds.) Scientific explanation (pp. 410-505). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Lewis, D. (1983). Mad pain and martian pain. In D. Lewis (Ed.) Philosophical papers, Vol. 1 (pp. 122-130). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Lipton, P. (2004). Inference to the best explanation. London, UK: Routledge.

(2007). Précis of Inference to the best explanation. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 74 (2), 421-423. 10.1111/j.1933-1592.2007.00027.x

Mareschal, I., Calder, A. J. & Clifford, C. W. G. (2013). Humans have an expectation that gaze is directed toward them. Current Biology, 23 (8), 717-721. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.030

Neander, K. (2012). Teleological theories of mental content. The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy, Spring 2012 Edition E. N. Zalta (Ed.) http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2012/entries/content-teleological/

Orlandi, N. (2013). Embedded seeing: Vision in the natural world. Noûs, 47 (4), 727-747. 10.1111/j.1468-0068.2011.00845.x

Palmer, C. J., Paton, B., Hohwy, J. & Enticott, P. G. (2013). Movement under uncertainty: The effects of the rubber-hand illusion vary along the nonclinical autism spectrum. Neuropsychologia, 51 (10), 1942-1951. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.020

Pearl, J. (1988). Probabilistic reasoning in intelligent systems: Networks of plausible inference. San Fransisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann Publishers.

Piccinini, G. & Scarantino, A. (2011). Information processing, computation, and cognition. Journal of Biological Physics, 37 (1), 1-38. 10.1007/s10867-010-9195-3

Piccinini, G. (2006). Computational explanation in neuroscience. Synthese, 153 (3), 343-353. 10.1007/s11229-006-9096-y

Ramachandran, V. S. & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain. London, UK: Fourth Estate.

Rao, R. P. N. & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2 (1), 79-87. 10.1038/4580

Sotiropoulos, G., Seitz, A. R. & Seriés, P. (2011). Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction. Current Biology, 21 (21), R883-R884. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.013

Spratling, M. W. (2008). Predictive coding as a model of biased competition in visual attention. Vision Research, 48 (12), 1391-1408. 10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.009

Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Harvard, MA: Harvard University Press.

Trommershäuser, J., Körding, K. & Landy, M. (Eds.) (2011). Sensory cue integration. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Van Doorn, G., Hohwy, J. & Symmons, M. (2014). Can you tickle yourself if you swap bodies with someone else? Consciousness and Cognition, 23, 1-11. 10.1016/j.concog.2013.10.009

von Helmholtz, H. (1867). Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. Leipzig, GER: Leopold Voss.