References

Ahmed, B., Cordery, P. M., McLelland, D., Bair, W. & Krug, K. (2012). Long-range clustered connections within extrastriate visual area V5/MT of the rhesus macaque. Cerebral Cortex, 22 (1), 60-73. 10.1093/cercor/bhr072

Albert, M. K. & Hoffman, D. D. (2000). The generic-viewpoint assumption and illusory contours. Perception, 29 (3), 303-312.

Alkire, M. T., Hudetz, A. G. & Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness and anesthesia. Science, 322 (5903), 876-80. 10.1126/science.1149213

Allman, J., Miezin, F. & McGuinness, E. (1985). Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: Neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 8, 407-30. 10.1146/annurev.ne.08.030185.002203

Almeida, J., Mahon, B. Z. Nakayama, K. & Caramazza, A. (2008). Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105 (39), 15214-15218. 10.1073/pnas.0805867105

Barbur, J. L. & Spang, K. (2008). Colour constancy and conscious perception of changes of illuminant. Neuropsychologia, 46 (3), 853-863. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.11.032

Barbur, J. L., de Cunha, D., Williams, C. B. & Plant, G. (2004). Study of instantaneous color constancy mechanisms in human vision. Journal of Electronic Imaging, 13 (1), 15-28. 10.1117/1.1636491

Bekinschtein, T. A., Dehaene, S., Rohaut, B., Tadel, F., Cohen, L. & Naccache, L. (2009). Neural signature of the conscious processing of auditory regularities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106 (5), 1672-1677. 10.1073/pnas.0809667106

Block, N. (1996). How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness? Trends in Neurosciences, 19 (11), 456-459. 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)20049-9

(2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (2), 46-52. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006

(2007). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30 (5-6), 481-499. 10.1017/S0140525X07002786

Bosking, W. H., Zhang, Y., Schofield, B. & Fitzpatrick, D. (1997). Orientation selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree shrew striate cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17 (6), 2112-2127.

Boyer, J. L., Harrison, S. & Ro, T. (2005). Unconscious processing of orientation and color without primary visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102 (46), 16875-16879. 10.1073/pnas.0505332102

Breitmeyer, B. G. & Ogmen, H. (2000). Recent models and findings in visual backward masking: A comparison, review, and update. Perception & Psychophysics, 62 (8), 1572-1595. 10.3758/BF03212157

Breitmeyer, B. G., Ogmen, H. G., Ro, T., Ogmen, H. & Todd, S (2007). Unconscious, stimulus-dependent priming and conscious, percept-dependent priming with chromatic stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 69 (4), 550-557. 10.3758/BF03193912

Breitmeyer, B. G., Ogmen, H.G., Ro, T. & Singhal, N. S. (2004). Unconscious color priming occurs at stimulus- not percept-dependent levels of processing. Psychological Science, 15 (3), 198-202. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503009.x

Bullier, J., Hupé, J. M., James, A. C. & Girard, P. (2001). The role of feedback connections in shaping the responses of visual cortical neurons. Progress in Brain Research, 134, 193-204.

Casali, A. G., Gosseries, O., Rosanova, M., Boly, M., Sarasso, S., Casali, K. R., Casarotto, S., Bruno, M. A., Laureys, S., Tononi, G. & Massimini, M. (2013). A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior. Science Translational Medicine, 5 (198), 198ra105. 10.1126/scitranslmed.3006294

Chalmers D. J. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2 (3), 200-219.

Chopin, A. & Mamassian, P. (2012). Predictive properties of visual adaptation. Current Biology, 22 (7), 622-626. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.021

Churchland, P. M. (1981). Elimative materialism and the propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy, 78 (2), 67-90. 10.2307/2025900

Cohen, M. A. & Dennett, D. C. (2011). Consciousness cannot be separated from function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15 (8), 358-364. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.06.008

Costello, P., Jiang, Y., Baartman, B., McGlennen, K. & He, S. (2009). Semantic and subword priming during binocular suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 18 (2), 375-382. 10.1016/j.concog.2009.02.003

Cowey, A. & Heywood, C. A. (1997). Cerebral achromatopsia: Colour blindness despite wavelength processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1 (4), 133-139. 10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01043-7

Cox, M. A., Schmid, M. C., Peters, A. J., Saunders, R. C., Leopold, D. A. & Maier, A. (2013). Receptive field focus of visual area V4 neurons determines responses to illusory surfaces. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 (42), 17095-17100. 10.1073/pnas.1310806110

Crick, F. & Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex, 8 (2), 97-107. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9542889

(2003). A framework for consciousness. Nature Neuroscience, 6 (2), 119-126. 10.1038/nn0203-119

Cumming, B. G. & Parker, A. J. (1997). Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception. Nature, 389 (6648), 280-283. 10.1038/38487

Dehaene, S. & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79 (1-2), 1-37. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11164022

Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J. & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10 (5), 204-211. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007

Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Naccache, L., Ciuciu, P., Poline, J. B., Le Bihan, D. & Cohen, L. (2004). Letter binding and invariant recognition of masked words: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Psychological Science, 15 (5), 307-313. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00674.x

Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec'H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Van de Moortele, P. F. & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395 (6702), 597-600. 10.1038/26967. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9783584

Del Cul, A., Baillet, S. & Dehaene, S. (2007). Brain dynamics underlying the nonlinear threshold for access to consciousness. PLoS Biology, 5 (10), e260. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050260

Dennett, D. C. (1993). Consciousness explained. New York, NY: Penguin. (pp. 889-892). 10.2307/2108259

Dow, B. M., Snyder, A. Z., Vautin, R. G. & Bauer, R. (1981). Magnification factor and receptive field size in foveal striate cortex of the monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 44 (2), 213-228. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7286109

Duncan, R. O. & Boynton, G. M. (2003). Cortical magnification within human primary visual cortex correlates with acuity thresholds. Neuron, 38 (4), 659-671. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00265-4

Edelman, D. B. & Seth, A. K. (2009). Animal consciousness: A synthetic approach. Trends in Neurosciences, 32 (9), 476-484. 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.008

Enns, J. T. & Di Lollo V. (2000). What's new in visual masking? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (9), 345-352. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01520-5

Fahrenfort, J. J. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2012). A true science of consciousness explains phenomenology: Comment on Cohen and Dennett. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16 (3), 138-140. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.004

Fahrenfort, J. J., Scholte, H. S. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19 (9), 1488-1497. 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1488

Fahrenfort, J. J., Snijders, T. M., Heinen, K., Van Gaal, S., Scholte, H. S. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2012). Neuronal integration in visual cortex elevates face category tuning to conscious face perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 (52), 21504-21509. 10.1073/pnas.1207414110

Faivre, N. & Koch, C. (2014). Temporal structure coding with and without awareness. Cognition, 131 (3), 404-414. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.008

Fang, F. & He, S. (2005). Cortical responses to invisible objects in the human dorsal and ventral pathways. Nature Neuroscience, 8 (10), 1380-1385. 10.1038/nn1537

Foster, K. H., Gaska, J. P., Nagler, M. & Pollen, D. A. (1985). Spatial and temporal frequency selectivity of neurones in visual cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. Journal of Physiology, 365, 331-363.

Freiwald, W. A. & Tsao, D. Y. (2010). Functional compartmentalization and viewpoint generalization within the macaque face-processing system. Science, 330 (6005), 845-851. 10.1126/science.1194908

Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6 (8), 653-659. 10.1038/nrn1723

Gilbert, C. D. & Wiesel, T. N. (1989). Columnar specificity of intrinsic horizontal and corticocortical connections in cat visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 9 (7), 2432-42. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2746337

(1992). Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex. Nature, 356 (6365), 150-2. 10.1038/356150a0. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1545866

Gilbert, C. D., Das, A., Ito, M., Kapadia, M. & Westheimer, G. (1996). Spatial integration and cortical dynamics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 93 (2), 615-622.

Gilbert, C. D., Sigman, M. & Crist, R. E. (2001). The neural basis of perceptual learning. Neuron, 31 (5), 681-97. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11567610

Girard, P., Hupé J. M., James A. C. & Bullier, J. (2001). Feedforward and feedback connections between areas V1 and V2 of the monkey have similar rapid conduction velocities. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85 (3), 1328-1331. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11248002

Gobbini, M. I., Gors, J. D., Halchenko, Y. O., Hughes, H. C. & Cipolli, C. (2013). Processing of invisible social cues. Consciousness and Cognition, 22 (3), 765-770. 10.1016/j.concog.2013.05.002

Goodale, M. A. & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15 (1), 20-25. 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

Gutnisky, D. A., Hansen, B. J., Iliescu, B. F. & Dragoi, V. (2009). Attention alters visual plasticity during exposure-based learning. Current Biology, 19 (7), 555-560. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.063

Harris, J. J., Schwarzkopf, D. S., Song, C., Bahrami, B. & Rees, G. (2011). Contextual illusions reveal the limit of unconscious visual vrocessing. Psychological Science, 22 (3), 399-405. 10.1177/0956797611399293

He, S., Cavanagh, P. & Intriligator, J (1996). Attentional resolution and the locus of visual awareness. Nature, 383 (6598), 334-337. 10.1038/383334a0

Hess, R. F., Beaudot, W. H. & Mullen, K. T. (2001). Dynamics of contour integration. Vision Research, 41 (8), 1023-1037. 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00020-7

Hubel, D. H. & Wiesel, T. N. (1968). Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex. Journal of Physiology, 195 (1), 215-243.

Hubel, D. H. (1982). Exploration of the primary visual cortex, 1955-78. Nature, 299 (5883), 515-524. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6750409

Hupé, J. M., James, A. C., Girard, P. & Bullier, J. (2001). Response modulations by static texture surround in area V1 of the macaque monkey do not depend on feedback connections from V2. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85 (1), 146-63.

Hupé, J. M., James, A. C., Girard, P., Lomber, S. G., Payne, B. R. & Bullier, J. (2001). Feedback connections act on the early part of the responses in monkey visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 85 (1), 134-145.

Hupé, J. M., James, A. C., Payne, B. R., Lomber, S. G., Girard, P. & Bullier, J. (1998). Cortical feedback improves discrimination between figure and background by V1, V2 and V3 neurons. Nature, 394 (6695), 784-787. 10.1038/29537

Hwang, E., Kim, S., Han, K. & Choi, J. H. (2012). Characterization of phase transition in the thalamocortical system during anesthesia-induced loss of consciousness. PLoS One, 7 (12), e50580. 10.1371/journal.pone.0050580

Jiang, Y., Costello, P., Fang, F., Huang, M. & He, S. (2006). A gender- and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103 (45), 17048-17052. 10.1073/pnas.0605678103

Kamermans, M., Kraaij, D. A. & Spekreijse, H. (1998). The cone/horizontal cell network: A possible site for color constancy. Visual Neuroscience, 15 (5), 787-797.

Kapadia, M. K., Ito, M., Gilbert, C. D. & Westheimer, G. (1995). Improvement in visual sensitivity by changes in local context: Parallel studies in human observers and in V1 of alert monkeys. Neuron, 15 (4), 843-856. 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90175-2

Karni, A. & Sagi, D. (1991). Where practice makes perfect in texture discrimination: Evidence for primary visual cortex plasticity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 11 (88), 4966-4970. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.063

Kim, C. Y. & Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: Rendering the visible 'invisible'. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (8), 381-388. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.012

King, J. R. & Dehaene, S. (2014). A model of subjective report and objective discrimination as categorical decisions in a vast representational space. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369 (1641), 1471-2970. 10.1098/rstb.2013.0204

King, J. R., Sitt, J. D., Faugeras, F., Rohaut, B., El Karoui, I., Cohen, L., Naccache, L. & Dehaene, S. (2013). Information sharing in the brain indexes consciousness in noncommunicative patients. Current Biology, 23 (19), 1914-1919. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.075. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24076243

Knierim, J. J. & Van Essen, D. C. (1992). Neuronal responses to static texture patterns in area V1 of the alert macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 67 (4), 961-980.

Koch, C. & Tsuchiya, N. (2012). Attention and consciousness: Related yet different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16 (2), 103-105. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.012

Kolb, F. C. & Braun, J. (1995). Blindsight in normal observers. Nature, 377 (6547), 336-338. 10.1038/377336a0

Kouider, S., Eger, E., Dolan, R. & Henson, R. N. (2009). Activity in face-responsive brain regions is modulated by invisible, attended faces: Evidence from masked priming. Cerebral Cortex, 19 (1), 13-23. 10.1093/cercor/bhn048

Kovács, I., Kozma, P., Fehér, A. & Benedek, G. (1999). Late maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96 (21), 12204-12209. 10.1073/pnas.96.21.12204

Lamme, V. A. F. & Roelfsema, P. R. (2000). The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends in Neurosciences, 23 (11), 571-9. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11074267

Lamme, V. A. F. & Spekreijse, H. (2000). Modulations of primary visual cortex activity representing attentive and conscious scene perception. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, D232-43. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10704153

Lamme, V. A. F. (1995). The neurophysiology of figure-ground segregation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 15 (2), 1605-1615.

(2003). Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (1), 12-18. 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X

(2004). Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention; a case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks, 17 (5-6), 861-872. 10.1016/j.neunet.2004.02.005

(2006). Towards a true neural stance on consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10 (11), 494-501. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.001

(2010a). How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1 (3), 204-220. 10.1080/17588921003731586

(2010b). What introspection has to offer, and where its limits lie. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1 (3), 232-240. 10.1080/17588928.2010.502224

Lamme, V. A. F., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, V. & Spekreijse, H. (1999). Separate processing dynamics for texture elements, boundaries and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 9 (4), 406-13. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10426419

Lamme, V. A. F., Supèr, H. & Spekreijse, H. (1998a). Feedforward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8 (4), 529-35. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9751656

Lamme, V. A. F., Supèr, H., Landman, R., Roelfsema, P. R. & Spekreijse, H. (2000). The role of primary visual cortex (V1) in visual awareness. Vision Research, 40 (10-12), 1507-21. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10788655

Lamme, V. A. F., Van Dijk, B. W. & Spekreijse, H. (1993). Contour from motion processing occurs in primary visual cortex. Nature, 363 (6429), 541-543. 10.1038/363541a0

Lamme, V. A. F., Zipser, K. & Spekreijse, H. (1998b). Figure-ground activity in primary visual cortex is suppressed by anesthesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95 (6), 3263-8. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9501251

(2002). Masking interrupts figure-ground signals in V1. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14 (7), 1044-53. 10.1162/089892902320474490. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12419127

Li, F. F., Van Rullen, R., Koch, C. & Perona, P. (2002). Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99 (14), 9596-9601. 10.1073/pnas.092277599

Macknik, S. L. & Livingstone, M. S. (1998). Neuronal correlates of visibility and invisibility in the primate visual system. Nature Neuroscience, 1 (2), 144-149. 10.1038/393

Maier J., Dagnelie G., Sprekreijse H. & Van Dijk B. W. (1987). Principal components-analysis for source localization of VEPs in man. Vision Research, 27 (2), 165-177. 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90179-9

Malach, R., Amir, Y., Harel, M. & Grinvald, A. (1993). Relationship between intrinsic connections and functional architecture revealed by optical imaging and in vivo targeted biocytin injections in primate striate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 90 (22), 10469-10473.

Maniscalco, B. & Lau, H. (2012). A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Consciousness and Cognition, 21 (1), 422-430. 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.021

Marcel, A. J. (1998). Blindsight and shape perception: Deficit of visual consciousness or of visual function? Brain, 121 (8), 1565-1588. 10.1093/brain/121.8.1565

Meng, M., Ferneyhough, E. & Tong, F. (2007). Dynamics of perceptual filling-in of visual phantoms revealed by binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 7 (13). 10.1167/7.13.8

Meuwese, J. D., Post, R. A., Scholte, H. S. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2013). Does perceptual learning require consciousness or attention? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25 (10), 1579-1596. 10.1162/jocn_a_00424

Meuwese, J. D., Scholte, H. S. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2014). Latent memory of unattended stimuli reactivated by practice: An FMRI study on the role of consciousness and attention in learning. PLoS One, 9 (3), e90098. 10.1371/journal.pone.0090098. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24603676

Moore, T., Rodman, H. R., Repp, A. B. & Gross, C. G. (1995). Localization of visual stimuli after striate cortex damage in monkeys: Parallels with human blindsight. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92 (18), 8215-8218. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7667270

Moradi, F., Koch, C. & Shimojo, S. (2005). Face adaptation depends on seeing the face. Neuron, 45 (1), 169-175. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.018

Moutoussis, K. & Zeki, S. (2002). The relationship between cortical activation and perception investigated with invisible stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99 (14), 9527-9532. 10.1073/pnas.142305699

Mudrik, L., Breska, A., Lamy, D. & Deouell, L. Y. (2011). Integration without awareness: Expanding the limits of unconscious processing. Psychological Science, 22 (6), 764-770. 10.1177/0956797611408736

Nakayama, K. & Shimojo, S. (1992). Experiencing and perceiving visual surfaces. Science, 257 (5075), 1357-1363.

Nothdurft, H. C., Gallant, J. L. & Van Essen, D. C. (1999). Response modulation by texture surround in primate area V1: Correlates of "popout" under anesthesia. Visual Neuroscience, 16 (1), 15-34.

Oram, M. W. & Perrett, D. I. (1992). Time course of neural responses discriminating different views of the face and head. Visual Neuroscience, 68 (1), 70-84.

Overgaard, M., Rote, J., Mouridsen, K. & Ramsøy, T. Z. (2006). Is conscious perception gradual or dichotomous? A comparison of report methodologies during a visual task. Consciousness and Cognition, 15 (4), 700-708. 10.1016/j.concog.2006.04.002

Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Boly, M., Davis, M. H., Laureys, S. & Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313 (5792), 1402. 10.1126/science.1130197

Panichello, M. F., Cheung, O. S. & Bar, M. (2012). Predictive feedback and conscious visual experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00620

Pascual-Leone, A. & Walsh, V. (2001). Fast backprojections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness. Science, 292 (5516), 510-512. 10.1126/science.1057099

Rao, R. P. & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2 (1), 79-87. 10.1038/4580

Robichaud, L. & Stelmach, L. B. (2003). Inducing blindsight in normal observers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10 (1), 206-209.

Rock, I. & Palmer, S. (1990). The legacy of Gestalt psychology. Scientific American, 263 (6), 84-90. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2270461

Roe, A. W., Lu, H. D. & Hung, C. P. (2005). Cortical processing of a brightness illusion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102 (10), 3869-3874. 10.1073/pnas.0500097102

Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). Cortical algorithms for perceptual grouping. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 29, 203-227. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112939

Roelfsema, P. R., Lamme, V. A. F., Spekreijse, H. & Bosch, H. (2002). Figure-ground segregation in a recurrent network architecture. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14 (4), 525-537. 10.1162/08989290260045756

Rolls, E. T. & Tovee, M. J. (1994). Processing speed in the cerebral cortex and the neurophysiology of visual masking. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 257 (1348), 9-15. 10.1098/rspb.1994.0087. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8090795

Rolls, E. T. (1992). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying face processing within and beyond the temporal cortical visual areas. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 335 (1273), 11-21. 10.1098/rstb.1992.0002

Rossi, A. F. & Paradiso, M. A. (1999). Neural correlates of perceived brightness in the retina, lateral geniculate nucleus, and striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 19 (14), 6145-56.

Rossi, A. F., Rittenhouse, C. D. & Paradiso, M. A. (1996). The representation of brightness in primary visual cortex. Science, 273 (5278), 1104-1107.

Scholte, H. S., Witteveen, S. C., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2006). The influence of inattention on the neural correlates of scene segmentation. Brain Research, 1076 (1), 106-115. 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.10.051

Schwiedrzik, C. M., Singer, W. & Melloni, L. (2011). Subjective and objective learning effects dissociate in space and in time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108 (11), 4506-4511. 10.1073/pnas.1009147108

Seghier, M. L. & Vuilleumier, P. (2006). Functional neuroimaging findings on the human perception of illusory contours. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30 (5), 595-612. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.11.002

Seitz, A. R. & Watanabe, T. (2003). Psychophysics: Is subliminal learning really passive? Nature, 422 (6927), 36. 10.1038/422036a

Seitz, A. R., Kim, D. & Watanabe, T. (2009). Rewards evoke learning of unconsciously processed visual stimuli in adult humans. Neuron, 61 (5), 700-707. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.01.016

Self, M. W., Kooijmans, R. N., Supèr, H., Lamme, V. A. F. & Roelfsema, P. R. (2012). Different glutamate receptors convey feedforward and recurrent processing in macaque V1. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109 (27), 11031-6. 10.1073/pnas.1119527109. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22615394

Self, M. W., Van Kerkoerle, T., Supèr, H. & Roelfsema, P. R. (2013). Distinct roles of the cortical layers of area V1 in figure-ground segregation. Current Biology, 23 (21), 2121-2129. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.013

Sergent, C. & Dehaene, S. (2004). Is consciousness a gradual phenomenon? Evidence for an all-or-none bifurcation during the attentional blink. Psychological Science, 15 (11), 720-728. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00748.x. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15482443

Seth, A. K. (2010). The grand challenge of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 1. 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00005

Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K. & Critchley, H. D. (2011). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 395. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22291673

Shapley, R. & Hawken, M. J. (2011). Color in the cortex: Single- and double-opponent cells. Vision Research, 51 (7), 701-717. 10.1016/j.visres.2011.02.012

Sigman, M. & Gilbert, C. D. (2000). Learning to find a shape. Nature Neuroscience, 3 (3), 264-269. 10.1038/72979

Sigman, M., Pan, H., Yang, Y., Stern, E., Silbersweig, D. & Gilbert, C. D. (2005). Top-down reorganization of activity in the visual pathway after learning a shape identification task. Neuron, 46 (5), 823-835. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.014

Silvanto, J., Lavie, N. & Walsh, V. (2005). Double dissociation of V1 and V5/MT activity in visual awareness. Cerebral Cortex, 15 (11), 1736-1741. 10.1093/cercor/bhi050

Simons, D. J. & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9 (1), 16-20. 10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.006. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15639436

Snodderly, D. M. & Gur, M. (1995). Organization of striate cortex of alert, trained monkeys (Macaca fascicularis): Ongoing activity, stimulus selectivity, and widths of receptive field activating regions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74 (5), 2100-2125.

Sperry, R. (1984). Consciousness, personal identity and the divided brain. Neuropsychologia, 22 (6), 661-673. 10.1016/0028-3932(84)90093-9

Stein, T., Hebart, M. N. & Sterzer, P. (2011). Breaking continuous flash suppression: A new measure of unconscious processing during interocular suppression? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5. 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00167

Steyn-Ross, M. L., Steyn-Ross, D. A., Sleigh, J. W. and Liley, D. T. (1999). Theoretical electroencephalogram stationary spectrum for a white-noise-driven cortex: Evidence for a general anesthetic-induced phase transition. Physical Review E - Statistical Physics, Plasmas, Fluids, and Related Interdisciplinary Topics, 60 (6 Pt B), 7299-7311. 10.1103/PhysRevE.60.7299

Stoerig, P. & Cowey, A. (1989). Wavelength sensitivity in blindsight. Nature, 342 (6252), 916-918. 10.1038/342916a0

Straube, T., Mothes-Lasch, M. & Miltner, W. H. (2011). Neural mechanisms of the automatic processing of emotional information from faces and voices. British Journal of Social Psychology, 102 (4), 830-848. 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02056.x

Sugase, Y., Yamane, S., Ueno, S. & Kawano, K. (1999). Global and fine information coded by single neurons in the temporal visual cortex. Nature, 400 (6747), 869-873. 10.1038/23703

Sugihara, T., Qiu, F. T. & Von der Heydt, R. (2011). The speed of context integration in the visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106 (1), 374-385. 10.1152/jn.00928.2010

Supèr, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2007). Altered figure-ground perception in monkeys with an extra-striate lesion. Neuropsychologia, 45 (14), 3329-3334. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.001

Supèr, H., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2001). Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Nature Neuroscience, 4 (3), 304-310. 10.1038/85170

Supèr, H., Van der Togt, C., Spekreijse, H. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Internal state of monkey primary visual cortex (V1) predicts figure-ground perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 23 (8), 3407-3414.

Swets, J. A., Green, D. M., Getty, D. J. & Swets, J. B. (1978). Signal detection and identification at successive stages of observation. Perception & Psychophysics, 23 (4), 275-289.

Sáry, G., Köteles, K., Kaposvári, P., Lenti, L., Csifcsák, G., Frankó, E., Benedek, G. & Tompa, T. (2008). The representation of Kanizsa illusory contours in the monkey inferior temporal cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28 (10), 2137-2146. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06499.x

Tanaka, K. (1996). Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 109-139. 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.000545. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8833438

Tononi, G. & Massimini, M. (2008). Why does consciousness fade in early sleep? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1129, 330-334. 10.1196/annals.1417.024

Tononi, G. (2004). An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neuroscience, 5 (42). 10.1186/1471-2202-5-42. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15522121

(2008). Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. Biological Bulletin, 215 (3), 216-242.

(2012). Integrated information theory of consciousness: an updated account. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 150 (4), 293-329.

Tsuchiya, N. & Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nature Neuroscience, 8 (8), 1096-1101. 10.1038/nn1500

Van der Togt, C., Kalitzin, S., Spekreijse, H., Lamme, V. A. F. & Supèr, H. (2006). Synchrony dynamics in monkey V1 predict success in visual detection. Cerebral Cortex, 16 (1), 136-148. 10.1093/cercor/bhi093

Van Gaal, S. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2012). Unconscious high-level information processing: Implication for neurobiological theories of consciousness. Neuroscientist, 18 (3), 287-301. 10.1177/1073858411404079

Van Loon, A. M., Fahrenfort, J. J., Van der Velde, B., Lirk, P. B., Vulink, N. C. C., Hollmann, M. W., Scholte, H. S. and Lamme, V. A. F. (submitted). NMDA receptor antagonist ketamine distorts object recognition by reducing feedback to early visual cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience

Von der Heydt, R., Peterhans, E. & Baumgartner, G. (1984). Illusory contours and cortical neuron responses. Science, 224 (4654), 1260-1262.

Wagemans, J., Elder, J. H., Kubovy, M., Palmer, S. E., Peterson, M. A., Singh, M. and Von der Heydt, R. (2012). A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. Psychological Bulletin, 138 (6), 1172-1217. 10.1037/a0029333

Wang, L., Weng, X. & He, S. (2012). Perceptual grouping without awareness: Superiority of Kanizsa triangle in breaking interocular suppression. PLoS One, 7 (6), e40106. 10.1371/journal.pone.0040106

Weiskrantz, L. (1996). Blindsight revisited. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6 (2), 215-220. 10.1016/S0959-4388(96)80075-4

Wokke, M. E., Sligte, I. G., Scholte, H. S. & Lamme, V. A. F. (2012). Two critical periods in early visual cortex during figure-ground segregation. Brain and Behavior, 2 (6), 763-777. 10.1002/brb3.91

Yamahachi, H., Marik, S. A., McManus, J. N., Denk, W. & Gilbert, C. D. (2009). Rapid axonal sprouting and pruning accompany functional reorganization in primary visual cortex. Neuron, 64 (5), 719-729. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.11.026

Zipser, K., Lamme, V. A. F. & Schiller, P. H. (1996). Contextual modulation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 16 (22), 7376-7389.