References

Adams, F. & Aizawa, K. (2001). The bounds of cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 14 (1), 43-64. 10.1080/09515080120033571

(2008). The bounds of cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

Adams, R. A., Perrinet, L. U. & Friston, K. (2012). Smooth pursuit and visual occlusion: Active inference and oculomotor control in schizophrenia. PLoS One, 7 (10), e47502. 10.1371/journal.pone. 0047502

Adams, R. A., Shipp, S. & Friston, K. J. (2013). Predictions not commands: Active inference in the motor system. Brain Structure and Function, 218 (3), 611-643. 10.1007/s00429-012-0475-5

Aertsen, A. & Preißl, H. (1991). Dynamics of activity and connectivity in physiological neuronal networks. In H. G. Schuster (Ed.) Nonlinear dynamics and neuronal networks (pp. 281-302). Weinheim, GER: VCH Verlag.

Anderson, M. L., Richardson, M. & Chemero, A. (2012). Eroding the boundaries of cognition: Implications of embodiment. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4 (4), 717-730. 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01211.x

Anscombe, G. E. M. (1957). Intention. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.

Ballard, D. (1991). Animate vision. Artificial Intelligence, 48, 57-86. 10.1016/0004-3702(91)90080-4

Ballard, D., Hayhoe, M., Pook, P. & Rao, R. (1997). Deictic codes for the embodiment of cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20 (4), 723-767.

Bastian, A. (2006). Learning to predict the future: The cerebellum adapts feedforward movement control. Current opinion in neurobiology, 16 (6), 645-649.

Bastos, A. M., Usrey, W. M., Adams, R. A., Mangun, G. R., Fries, P. & Friston, K. J. (2012). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron, 76 (4), 695-711. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038

Bastos, A. M., Vezoli, J., Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J.-M., Oostenveld, R., Dowdall, J. R., De Weerd, P., Kennedy, H. & Fries, P. (2015). Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018

Beer, R. D. (2000). Dynamical approaches to cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (3), 91-99. 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01440-0

Brooks, R. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139-159. 10.1.1.12.1680

Brown, H., Adams, R. A., Parees, I., Edwards, M. & Friston, K. (2013). Active inference, sensory attenuation and illusions. Cognitive Processing, 14 (4), 411-427. 10.1007/s10339-013-0571-3

Chapman, S. (1968). Catching a baseball. American Journal of Physics, 36, 868-870.

Churchland, P. S., Ramachandran, V. S. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1994). A critique of pure vision. In C. Koch & J. L. Davis (Eds.) Large Scale Neuronal Theories of the Brain (pp. 23-60). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Clark, A. & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 58 (1), 7-19. 10.1111/1467-8284.00096

Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10 (8), 370-374. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.012

(2008). Supersizing the mind: Action, embodiment, and cognitive extension. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

(2013a). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36 (3), 181-204. 10.1017/S0140525X12000477

(2013b). The many faces of precision. Frontiers in Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 4 (270), 1-9. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00270

(in press). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Conway, C. & Christiansen, M. (2001). Sequential learning in non-human primates. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5 (12), 539-546. 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01800-3

Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-based influences on humans' choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron, 69, 1204-1215. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.02

Dayan, P. & Daw, N. D. (2008). Decision theory, reinforcement learning, and the brain. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8 (4), 429-453. 10.3758/CABN.8.4.429

Dayan, P. (2012). How to set the switches on this thing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22 (6), 1068-1074. 10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.011

Edwards, M. J., Adams, R. A., Brown, H., Pareés, I. & Friston, K. (2012). A Bayesian account of 'hysteria'. Brain, 135 (11), 3495-3512. 10.1093/brain/aws129

Egner, T., Monti, J. M. & Summerfield, C. (2010). Expectation and surprise determine neural population responses in the ventral visual stream. Journal of Neuroscience, 30 (49), 16601-16608. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2770-10.2010

Estany, A. & Sturm, T. (Eds.) (2014). Extended cognition: New philosophical perspectives. Special Issue of Philosophical Psychology, 27 (1)

Feldman, H. & Friston, K. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4 (215), 1-23. 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215

Feldman, A. G. (2009). New insights into action-perception coupling. Experimental Brain Research, 194 (1), 39-58. 10.1007/s00221-008-1667-3

Felleman, D. J. & Van Essen, D. C. (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 1 (1), 1-47. 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a

Fink, P. W., Foo, P. S. & Warren, W. H. (2009). Catching fly balls in virtual reality: A critical test of the outfielder problem. Journal of Vision, 9 (13), 1-8. 10.1167/9.13.14

Flash, T. & Hogan, N. (1985). The coordination of arm movements: An experimentally confirmed mathematical model. The Journal of Neuroscience, 5 (7), 1688-1703. 10.1.1.134.529

Fletcher, P. & Frith, C. (2009). Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian appraoch to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 48-58. 10.1038/nrn2536

Franklin, D. W. & Wolpert, D. M. (2011). Computational mechanisms of sensorimotor control. Neuron, 72 (3), 425-442. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006

Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 29, 360 (1456), 815-836. 10.1098/rstb.2005.1622

(2008). Hierarchical models in the brain. PLoS Computational Biology, 4 (11), e1000211. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000211

(2009). The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13 (7), 293-301. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005

(2011). What is optimal about motor control? Neuron, 72 (3), 488-498. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.018

Friston, K., Adams, R. A., Perrinet, L. & Breakspear, M. (2012). Perceptions as hypotheses: Saccades as experiments. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (151), 1-20. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00151

Friston, K. J., Daunizeau, J., Kilner, J. & Kiebel, S. J. (2010). Action and behavior: A free-energy formulation. Biological Cybernetics, 102 (3), 227-260. 10.1007/s00422-010-0364-z

Friston, K., Mattout, J. & Kilner, J. (2011). Action understanding and active inference. Biological Cybernetics, 104 (1-2), 137-160. 10.1007/s00422-011-0424-z

Friston, K., Samothrakis, S. & Montague, R. (2012). Active inference and agency: Optimal control without cost functions. Biological Cybernetics, 106 (8-9), 523-541. 10.1007/s00422-012-0512-8

Friston, K. J., Shiner, T., Fitzgerald, T., Galea, J. M., Adams, R., Brown, H., Dolan, R. J., Moran, R., Stephan, K. E. & Bestmann, S. (2012). Dopamine, affordance and active inference. PLoS Computational Biology, 8 (1), e1002327. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002327

Frith, C. D. & Friston, K. J. (2012). False perceptions and false beliefs: Understanding schizophrenia. Working Group on Neurosciences and the Human Person: New Perspectives on Human Activities, The Pontifical academy of Sciences, 8-10 November 2012. Vatican City, VA: Casina Pio IV.

Gläscher, J., Daw, N., Dayan, P. & O'Doherty, J. P. (2010). States versus rewards: Dissociable neural prediction error signals underlying model based and model-free reinforcement learning. Neuron, 66 (4), 585-595. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.04.016

Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27 (3), 377-442. 10.1017/S0140525X04000093

Haruno, M., Wolpert, D. M. & Kawato, M. (2003). Hierarchical MOSAIC for movement generation. International congress series, 1250, 575-590.

Hinton, G. E. & Salakhutdinov, R. R. (2006). Reducing the dimensionality of data with neural networks. Science, 313 (5786), 504-507. 10.1126/science.1127647

Hinton, G. E., Osindero, S. & Teh, Y. (2006). A fast learning algorithm for deep belief nets. Neural Computation, 18 (7), 1527-1554. 10.1162/neco.2006.18.7.1527

Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

(2014). The neural organ explains the mind. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.) Open MIND. Frankfurt a. M., GER: MIND Group.

Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

(2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27 (1), 34-49. 10.1080/09515089.2013.830548

James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology Vol. I, II. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Jordan, M. I. & Jacobs, R. A. (1994). Hierarchical mixtures of experts and the EM algorithm. Neural Computation, 6 (2), 181-214. 10.1162/neco.1994.6.2.181

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. London, UK: Penguin.

Kawato, K. (1999). Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 9 (6), 718-727. 10.1016/S0959-4388(99)00028-8

Land, M. (2001). Does steering a car involve perception of the velocity flow field? In J. M. Zanker & J. Zeil (Eds.) Motion vision - Computational, neural, and ecological constraints (pp. 227-238). Berlin, GER: Springer Verlag.

Landy, D. & Goldstone, R. L. (2005). How we learn about things we don't already understand. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 17 (4), 343-369. 10.1080/09528130500283832

Littman, M., Majercik, S. & Pitassi, T. (2001). Stochastic Boolean satisfiability. Journal of Automated Reasoning, 27 (3), 251-296.

Lotze, H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, GER: Weidmannsche Buchhandlung.

Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational approach. SanFrancisco, CA: Freeman & Co.

Maturana, H. & Varela, F. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. Boston, MA: Reidel.

McClelland, J. L. (2013). Integrating probabilistic models of perception and interactive neural networks: A historical and tutorial review. Frontiers in Psychology, 4 (503), 1-25. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00503

Menary, R. (Ed.) (2010). The extended mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Mohan, V. & Morasso, P. (2011). Passive motion paradigm: An alternative to optimal control. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 5 (4), 1-28. 10.3389/fnbot.2011.00004

Mohan, V., Morasso, P., Metta, G. & Kasderidis, S. (2010). Actions & imagined actions in cognitive robots. In V. Cutsuridis, A. Hussain & J. G. Taylor (Eds.) Perception-reason-action cycle: Models, architectures, and hardware (pp. 1-32). New York, NY: Springer Series in Cognitive and Neural Systems.

Namikawa, J. & Tani, J. (2010). Learning to imitate stochastic time series in a compositional way by chaos. Neural Networks, 23 (5), 625-638. 10.1016/j.neunet.2009.12.006

Namikawa, J., Nishimoto, R. & Tani, J. (2011). A neurodynamic account of spontaneous behaviour. PLoS Computational Biology, 7 (10), e100222. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002221

Park, J. C., Lim, J. H., Choi, H. & Kim, D. S. (2012). Predictive coding strategies for developmental neurorobotics. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (134), 1-10. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00134

Pellicano, E. & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes too real: A Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16 (10), 504-510. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009

Pezzulo, G. (2008). Coordinating with the future: The anticipatory nature of representation. Minds and Machines, 18, 179-225. 10.1007/s11023-008-9095-5

(2014). Why do you fear the bogeyman? An embodied predictive coding model of perceptual inference. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14 (3), 902-911. 10.3758/s13415-013-0227-x

Pezzulo, G., Barsalou, L., Cangelosi, A., Fischer, M., McRae, K. & Spivey, M. (2013). Computational grounded cognition: A new alliance between grounded cognition and computational modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 (612), 1-11. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00612

Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F. & Chersi, F. (2013). The mixed instrumental controller: Using value of information to combine habitual choice and mental simulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 4 (92), 1-15. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00092

Poeppel, D. & Monahan, P. J. (2011). Feedforward and feedback in speech perception: Revisiting analysis by synthesis. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26 (7), 935-951. 10.1080/01690965.2010.493301

Price, C. J. & Devlin, J. T. (2011). The interactive Account of ventral occipito-temporal contributions to reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15 (6), 246-253. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.04.001

Raichle, M. E. & Snyder, A. Z. (2007). A default mode of brain function: A brief history of an evolving idea. NeuroImage, 37 (4), 1083-1090. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.02.041

Rao, R. & Ballard, D. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2 (1), 79-87. 10.1038/4580

Roepstorff, A. (2013). Interactively human: Sharing time, constructing materiality: Commentary on Clark. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36 (3), 224-225. 10.1017/S0140525X12002427

Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J. & Beck, S. (2010). Enculturating brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks, 23, 1051-1059. 10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.002

Roth, M. J., Synofzik, M. & Lindner, A. (2013). The cerebellum optimizes perceptual predictions about external sensory events. Current Biology, 23 (10), 930-935. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.027

Rupert, R. (2004). Challenges to the hypothesis of extended cognition. Journal of Philosophy, 101 (8), 389-428.

(2009). Cognitive systems and the extended mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Saegusa, R., Sakka, S., Metta, G. & Sandini, G. (2008). Sensory prediction learning - how to model the self and environment. Annecy, FR: The 12th IMEKO TC1-TC7 joint Symposium on "Man Science and Measurement" (IMEKO2008).

Seth, A. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17 (11), 565-573. 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007

Shaffer, D. M., Krauchunas, S. M., Eddy, M. & McBeath, M. K. (2004). How dogs navigate to catch frisbees. Psychological Science, 15 (7), 437-441. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00698.x

Shea, N. (2013). Perception vs. action: The computations may be the same but the direction of fit differs: Commentary on Clark. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36 (3), 228-229. 10.1017/S0140525X12002397

Shipp, S., Adams, R. A. & Friston, K. J. (2013). Reflections on agranular architecture: Predictive coding in the motor cortex. Trends in Neurosciences, 36 (12), 706-716. 10.1016/j.tins.2013.09.004

Smith, L. & Gasser, M. (2005). The development of embodied cognition: Six lessons from babies. Artificial Life, 11, 13-29. 10.1162/1064546053278973

Sommer, M. A. & Wurtz, R. H. (2006). Influence of thalamus on spatial visual processing in frontal cortex. Nature, 444 (7117), 374-377. 10.1038/nature05279

(2008). Brain circuits for the internal monitoring of movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 31 (1), 317-338. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627

Spivey, M. J. (2007). The continuity of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Spratling, M. W. (2008). Predictive coding as a model of biased competition in visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 48 (12), 1391-1408. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125627

(2010). Predictive coding as a model of response properties in cortical area V1. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30 (9), 3531-3543. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4911-09.2010

(2013). Distinguishing theory from implementation in predictive coding accounts of brain function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36 (3), 231-232.

Stigler, J. W. (1984). "Mental abacus": The effect of abacus training on Chinese children mental calculation. Cognitive Psychology, 16 (2), 145-176. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(84)90006-9

Tani, J. (2007). On the interactions between top-down anticipation and bottom-up regression. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 1 (2), 2. 10.3389/neuro.12.002.2007

Thelen, E. & Smith, L. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Massachusetts, MA: MIT Press.

Todorov, E. & Jordan, M. I. (2002). Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nature Neuroscience, 5 (11), 1226-1235. 10.1038/nn963

Todorov, E. (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nature Neuroscience, 7 (9), 907-915. 10.1038/nn1309

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The ontogeny and phylogeny of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28 (5), 675-691. 10.1017/S0140525X05000129

Uno, Y., Kawato, M. & Suzuki, R. (1989). Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement. Biological Cybernetics, 61 (2), 89-101. 10.1007/BF00204593

Von Holst, E. (1954). "Relations between the central Nervous System and the peripheral organs". The British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 2 (3), 89-94. 10.1016/S0950-5601(54)80044-X

Wolpert, D. M. & Kawato, M. (1998). Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Networks, 11 (7-8), 1317-1329. 10.1016/S0893-6080(98)00066-5

Wolpert, M. & Miall, D. M. (1996). Forward Models for Physiological Motor Control. Neural Networks, 9 (8), 1265-1279.

Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K. & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 358 (1431), 593-602. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1238

Wurtz, R. H., McAlonan, K., Cavanaugh, J. & Berman, R. A. (2011). Thalamic pathways for active vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15 (4), 177-184. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.02.004

Yamashita, Y. & Tani, J. (2008). Emergence of functional hierarchy in a multiple timescale neural network model: a humanoid robot experiment. PLoS ONE, 6 (10), e1000220. 10.1371/annotation/c580e39c-00bc-43a2-9b15-af71350f9d43

Zorzi, M., Testolin, A. & Stoianov, I. (2013). Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning: a tutorial overview. Frontiers Psychology, 4 (415), 1-14. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00515